|
|
|
Go Back
Implications of Interpersonal Neurobiology for a Spirituality of Compassion
A version of this essay is forthcoming in Zygon: Journal of Religion and Science. Parts of the paper were presented at the joint annual meeting of the Society for the Study of Psychology and Wesleyan Theology and the Wesleyan Philosophical Society at Duke University in March 2008. I am grateful to Gregory Johanson for introducing me to interpersonal neurobiology. For their very helpful comments on earlier drafts, I thank David Hogue, Joan D. Koss-Chioino, John McCarthy, Michael Schuck, LeRon Shults, and Ryan Cumming.
Introduction In the concluding summary of their important volume entitled Altruism and Altruistic Love, editors Stephen G. Post and Lynn G. Underwood spell out what they see as future research needs in the “science of altruism.” In the domain of spirituality and religion, they indicate that one set of questions for future research lies at the intersection of spiritual practices and altruistic love: What specific spiritual practices (e.g., types of prayer, meditation, silence, worship) might help to encourage altruistic love? How do these practices interact with the biological, social, and cultural substrate of the person?1 Since this volume’s release in 2002, significant scientific advances have been made that can aid in the search for provisional answers to these sorts of questions. Interpersonal neurobiology (IPNB), a term coined by UCLA psychiatrist Daniel J. Siegel, is a growing transdisciplinary field that focuses on ways in which relationships fundamentally shape and change the architecture and functioning of the human brain. In this essay, I argue that IPNB points to a specific set of scientifically-demonstrated conditions that appear to encourage the emergence of empathy; and further, that this set of conditions constitutes the core components of a “spirituality of compassion” by which specific spiritual practices in diverse religious traditions can be evaluated for their potential to cultivate caring attitudes and actions in selves and societies. Following introductory discussions on definitional and methodological issues, I present key assumptions in IPNB, and demonstrate ways in which IPNB sheds light on important aspects of human empathy and compassion. Then, drawing on this analysis, I introduce four specific conditions that appear to have profound potential to encourage the emergence of empathy in individuals and groups, and suggest that these criteria may function as central elements of a spirituality of compassion. Next, to demonstrate how this set of conditions might function, I offer a case study in which I describe the Native American Ojibwe practice of the “talking circle,” and assess it through the lens of my IPNB-derived spirituality of compassion. I conclude by addressing some questions that remain unanswered, and by suggesting areas for future research. Definitions Since the words “spirituality,” “empathy,” and “compassion” often mean quite different things to different scholars in different fields, it makes sense to begin by clarifying what is implied by them in the context of this essay. In this section, I offer what I judge to be the most helpful definitions of each term, given the goals and purview of the present study. Spirituality It is well known that “spirituality” defies definitional consensus. Lucy Bregman has recently argued that the current abundance of definitions for spirituality renders the concept too ambiguous to be coherent or meaningful.2 Yet, despite its annoying inexactness, spirituality seems to represent an important, abiding, “multilevel-multidimentional” phenomenon of human experience that touches on manifold aspects of life, including the socio-cultural, intellectual, emotional, behavioral, neurobiological, and existential.3 For these reasons and others, it continues to be studied, defined, and redefined by scholars from a wide range of disciplines. The plethora of definitions need not necessarily be negative; it may be that the array is due more to the diversity of cultures and traditions in which spirituality finds expression, and the variety of disciplines that examine it, than to any inherent murkiness in the construct itself. Whatever the case, the fact that spirituality’s meaning is somewhat of a moving goalpost ought not to deter us from attempts to understand and communicate its significance, and outline its contours in particular contexts. I am inclined to favor relational definitions of spirituality—in other words, those which tend to the dynamics of our ongoing relationships with ourselves, others, and that which we deem “sacred.” Following theologian F. LeRon Shults and psychologist Steven J. Sandage, I define spirituality as “ways of relating to the sacred.”4 The phrase “ways of relating” may include an array of relational postures—for example, “searching for,” “abiding within,” or “hiding from.” The term “sacred” may refer to “a divine being, divine object, Ultimate Reality, or Ultimate Truth as perceived by the individual,”5 and may also include persons, rituals, objects, narratives, texts, times, and spaces that are “set apart…as special, uniquely transcendent, and not ordinary or profane.”6 Defining spirituality as “ways of relating to the sacred” has three main advantages in the context of this essay. First, the emphasis on relationality can be utilized to highlight the ways in which interpersonal relationships and representations of the sacred reciprocally influence each other.7 Second, it can support a focus on the dynamism of spirituality; that is, the ongoing transformations in our ways of relating to the sacred throughout life. Third, it is an inclusive and versatile definition, and can function descriptively in relation to a wide range of spiritual practices.8 Empathy The English word “empathy” is a translation of the German Einfühlung, which literally means “in-feeling” or “feeling into.” The idea was first presented in 1873 by Robert Vischer as a term used in aesthetics. In 1903, empathy entered the field of psychology through the work of philosopher Theodore Lipps, who held that empathy is an “objectified enjoyment of self.” 9 Contemporary definitions of empathy often vary according to discipline. Philosopher Susan Langer speaks of empathy as an involuntary breach of individual separateness.10 Primatologist Frans De Wall defines it as “the ability to be affected by the state of another individual or creature.”11 Among social and developmental psychologists, definitions of empathy tend to center on emotional/affective responses to others.12 Neuroscientific definitions generally touch on both the “emotional” and “cognitive” aspects of empathy. Neuroscientists often emphasize that a central aspect of the experience and expression of empathy is the complex ability to make hypotheses about the state of the other while holding onto an awareness of the self.13 Interpersonal neurobiologists propose that empathy involves the complex process of imagining what it is like to be the other while simultaneously holding our own perspective in mind.14 I define empathy as the capacity to be affected by and share in the state of an other (or others) in such a way that we maintain self-awareness even as we “feel into” the other’s experience. Like many social-psychological accounts of empathy, my definition provides room for spontaneous response; we are often automatically (and/or unconsciously) “affected by” the states of others. It also echoes the neurobiological emphasis on knowledge of self and other as integral to empathy. Further, I wish to emphasize that my conception of “sharing in” does not imply “fusing with” the other. Rather, it means allowing the other’s experience to become part of my own in such a way that I am not (on the one hand) engulfed by the other, nor (on the other hand) am I cut off from the other; instead, I relate to the other in a self-differentiated15 manner. Compassion “Compassion” is derived from the Latin pati, meaning “to suffer,” and cum, meaning “with.” Thus, translated literally, compassion means “to suffer with.” As with empathy, definitions of compassion differ according to the contexts, perspectives, and interests of those doing the defining; however, most definitions hew closely to the word’s original etymological meaning. For Roman Catholic priest and spiritual writer Henri Nouwen, compassion means “going directly to those people and places where suffering is most acute and building a home there.”16 The Dalai Lama believes that human beings are “fundamentally compassionate;” that is, we have a “natural ability to connect spontaneously and deeply with the suffering of others.”17 Clinical psychologists Patrick R. Steffen and Kevin S. Masters speak of compassion as “being moved by the suffering of others and having the desire to alleviate that suffering,” and posit that a “compassionate personality” involves “altruistic behavior with a deep sense of empathy for the needs of others.”18 Like Steffen and Masters, neuroscientist John A. King and his colleagues add the important component of helping action to their definition; in their study, compassion was “exemplified by the Good Samaritan-like behavior of helping an unknown victim encountered on a single occasion.”19 This brief sampling of definitions reveals that sharing in the suffering of others, and wanting them to have respite, is fundamental to compassion. I define compassion as being empathically connected with others in their suffering, and taking action to ease their distress. This definition presupposes empathy as I have defined it above; however, it also goes beyond empathy in that it involves a component of action, or helping behavior.20 I agree with the Dalai Lama that compassion should not be confused with pity, which can imply that the sufferer is inferior to oneself.21 Rather, compassion is undergirded by a deep sense of respect for the other person. Finally, compassion should not be simply equated with all forms of “prosocial” action; helping behaviors are sometimes carried out in non-empathic, non-compassionate ways. While gauging human motives is always a thorny undertaking, as a general rule, a sense of empathic resonance with the pain of the other—a basic experience of “suffering with”—should be involved to some degree in those individual and communal expressions of care that we label “compassionate.”22 Putting these three definitions together, by “spirituality of compassion” I mean a way of relating to the sacred that cultivates empathic connectedness with others in their suffering and promotes action to ease their distress. A Postfoundationalist Transdisciplinary Approach I follow a postfoundationalist model for relating IPNB and spirituality. While it has been spelled out in several different ways,23 in each of its forms, postfoundationalist models emphasize that because all forms of human inquiry (including scientific and spiritual reflection) are irreducibly contextual and social, transdisciplinarity is a practical, embodied skill of particular, historically-situated, persons-in-relation. Practically speaking, postfoundationalism asks transdisciplinarians to assume self-aware, critical postures toward patterns in their traditions, beliefs, cultures, practices, and assumptions, and attempt to make sense of those patterns through ongoing dialogue with scholars in other fields. I take seriously the postfoundationalist imperatives to integrate self-awareness and communal dialogue into the core of the transdisciplinary task. For the present study, this means that I remain intentionally conscious of the ways in which my own experience as a person who has received graduate education in both psychotherapy and theology has birthed and continues to shape my approach to questions of human nature, compassion, and transformation. In constructing my arguments, I draw not only on scholarly sources, but also implicitly from my experiences working with patients in therapeutic settings, and from my critical self-reflections on those experiences. My thinking has also been enriched by innumerable transdisciplinary conversations I have had over the years with therapists, supervisors, and religious scholars. A postfoundationalist approach, therefore, gives me latitude to intentionally integrate my personal and vocational history, self-consciousness, and relations with others into the very heart of my work. In addition, postfoundationalism affirms that as we attempt to cross the boundaries of our particular disciplines and traditions, specific meeting-points for mutual understanding and collaboration can emerge. These “transversal spaces”—a notion originating in the work of Calvin Schrag24—are spaces in which different voices are not in opposition, nor in danger of becoming unified, but are dynamically engaged with one another. Transversality occurs when different disciplinary perspectives can lie across, extend over, intersect, meet, and convey without becoming fused or enmeshed.25 Compassion, I submit, may be thought of as one such “transversal space” in which the distinct assumptions and thought patterns of hard science and spiritual practice can meet. Basic Assumptions of Interpersonal Neurobiology Drawing richly from many different disciplines (including neuroscience, psychiatry, developmental psychology, social psychology, psychoanalysis, family systems theory, ethology, evolutionary theory, comparative anatomy, and genetics), IPNB aims to paint a picture of human experience and the dynamics of change across the lifespan by focusing on ways in which human beings are formed and transformed through relationships.26 IPNB is especially attentive to the processes by which neural systems shape human patterns of attachment, and, correlatively, how those attachment patterns shape neural systems. Louis Cozolino, Professor of Psychology at Pepperdine University and a key figure (with Daniel J. Siegel) in this bourgeoning field, defines IPNB simply as “the study of how we attach and grow and interconnect throughout life.”27 Prior to discussing specific ways in which IPNB sheds light on conditions that encourage the emergence of empathy and thereby opens up space in which to propose a scientifically informed spirituality of compassion, it will be necessary to outline several of IPNB’s basic assumptions. Brain and Mind First, IPNB assumes that interpersonal relationships are the natural habitat of the human brain and mind. Cozolino describes the brain as “an organ of adaptation that builds its structures through interactions with others.”28 The brain is a dynamic interpersonal system; just as neurons29 are constituted by their ongoing synaptic connections with other neurons, so too are brains continually being formed and re-formed through ongoing interactions with other brains. Because the brain is best described as an open system that undergoes continuous change in relational contexts across the lifespan,30 the concept of a “single brain” is a misnomer: Scientists have had to expand their thinking to grasp this idea: The individual neuron or a single human brain does not exist in nature. Without mutually stimulating interactions, people and neurons wither and die. In neurons this process is called apoptosis; in humans it is called depression, grief, and suicide… Thus, understanding the brain requires knowledge of the healthy, living brain embedded within a community of other brains.”31 Not only the brain’s health and vitality—but also its very existence—is essentially dependent upon the myriad relational connections that occur across “social synapses.”32 While Cozolino’s approach to IPNB focuses on the language of “the social brain,” Siegel’s method is marked by more direct concentration on the language of “mind,” which he defines as “a process that regulates the flow of energy and information.”33 For Siegel, the human mind is both neurobiological (involving the flow of energy and information within the body, including the brain) and interpersonal (involving the flow of energy and information between persons). Because “Energy and information can flow within one brain, or between brains,” the mind is said to emerge at the dynamic interface of embodied and relational processes.34 Thus, whether the language is centered on the human social brain and the social synapses that connect brains (Cozolino), or on the emergent processes of the human mind that include neurobiological and interpersonal processes (Siegel), IPNB places face-to-face, voice-to-voice communication at the heart of the human experience of reality. Social and Emotional Neurocircuitry Second, IPNB assumes that certain brain structures and systems—especially those involving the prefrontal cortex and limbic system—are particularly important for understanding the complex interactions between relationships, neurobiology, and personality development. The classic 1848 case of Phineas Gage—a railroad worker whose personality was significantly altered after an accidental explosion shot a four-foot tamping iron rod through his frontal lobes—is a symbolic marker of the rise of scientific interest in the link between biology and behavior in general, and the brain’s frontal lobes in particular. The frontal lobes evolved as humans became primates, and among primate species, humans have the most highly developed prefrontal cortices. Located (roughly) behind the forehead and eye area, the prefrontal cortex interacts with other systems of the brain to guide our emotional, social, and moral ways of being in the world. Prefrontal systems mediate many of the functions considered unique to homo sapiens, including the ability to regulate body systems, balance emotions, modulate fear, respond flexibly, exhibit insight, feel empathy, experience intuition, and make moral decisions.35 “An intact and well-developed prefrontal cortex enables us to maintain a simultaneous sense of self and others that is necessary for interpersonal strategizing and decision making.”36 In addition to a general interest in the prefrontal cortex, IPNB focuses on specific cortical and subcortical structures of the brain that are important for understanding social and emotional processes. Many of these structures are considered part of the limbic system, which is closely linked with the prefrontal cortex and supports a variety of emotional, behavioral, and memory functions. Right hemispheric limbic brain areas are integrally involved in socioemotional processing and self-regulation.37 Let us briefly consider six of the most important structures of the social brain—many of which are hidden beneath the brain’s surface. 38 - The cingulate cortex is associated with long-term emotional bonds, social cooperation, and empathy.
- The orbital medial prefrontal cortex (OMPFC), anatomically perched at the top of the centrally located limbic system, is responsible for integrating sensory (external) and emotional (internal) information with motivation and reward systems in the guiding of perceptions, actions, and interactions.
- The hypothalamus is involved in linking conscious experience and bodily processes (such as hunger and thirst), and also regulates sexual behavior and aggression.
- The insula cortex appears to mediate a vast range of emotions (from intense disgust to passionate love) and, with the anterior cingulate, allows for awareness of bodily states and reflections on emotional experiences.
- The hippocampus specializes in organizing spatial, sequential, and emotional learning and memory.
- The amygdala plays a central role in fear responses; it specializes in rapidly appraising danger and initiating automatic fight/flight reactions to threat.
In addition to these (and other) cortical and subcortical structures, the sensory, motor, and affective systems, as well as the regulatory systems,39 are central facets of the social brain. As we will see, many of the neurological structures and systems that IPNB finds indispensable for understanding human social and emotional life play a central role in the experience and expression of compassion. At the same time that IPNB singles out discrete neurological structures and systems, it also attends to the connections between them in order to better understand the dynamic processes by which the brain regulates the flow of energy and information. The linking up of neural structures and networks in ways that that contribute to the establishment of “a functional flow in the states of mind across time”40 is known as “neural integration,”41 and the middle prefrontal cortex is generally seen as the main hub of this process. Greater levels of neural integration are associated with increased capacities to balance emotion, construct coherent life narratives, experience self-awareness, respond adaptively to stress, form meaningful relationships with others, regulate the body, and (most importantly for our purposes) respond empathically to others. As we will see, there are specific conditions that appear to encourage the process of neural integration. Attachment Theory Third, IPNB assumes that attachment theory provides the best available model for analyzing the interaction between relationships and the brain in the unfolding of the human personality. Originating in the 1950’s in the work of British psychoanalyst John Bowlby,42 attachment theory seeks to understand the human person in the context of his or her ongoing embeddedness in close, emotional relationships with significant others. Attachment theory postulates the existence of an inborn, evolutionarily-formed attachment system that enhances infant survival by recruiting relational resources from the primary caregiver in order to successfully regulate anxiety and fear in the face of perceived threat. As the child grows, early relational patterns become internalized dispositions (“internal working models of attachment”) that profoundly and implicitly shape the way the individual relates to others (and to the self) throughout life. In other words, early relationships become “hard-wired” in the brain, and later function as unconscious “templates” that structure relationships in adult life—for good or ill. In recent decades, attachment theory has garnered significant empirical support and has gained acceptance in a wide range of social-scientific fields. Overall, it has proven a reliable model for analyzing not only infant-caregiver bonds, but also romantic bonds in adult life.43 Many controlled studies have confirmed its usefulness for explaining and predicting various aspects of human experience over the lifespan, including adult behavioral tendencies, developmental patterns, relational styles, cognitive processes, and self-regulatory abilities.44 By observing hundreds of primary caregiver-infant dyads in controlled settings, early attachment researchers45 proposed the existence of three main bonding styles (or “attachment schemas”): secure, avoidant, and anxious-ambivalent. Secure attachment is promoted by sensitive, consistent, and responsive care from the primary caregiver. A secure attachment bond nurtures infant development by providing a “secure base” for environmental exploration, a reliable relational context for learning how to adaptively regulate cognitive and affective processes in the face of anxiety, and an orientation to human relationships marked by a balance of separateness and connectedness. Insecure attachment bonds “bias the infant’s relational development toward either an excessive preoccupation with maintaining proximity or toward a persistent avoidance of closeness.”46 Avoidant attachment is associated with dismissive parenting, and contributes to a “deactivation” of the infant’s attachment system. Since they have learned to expect indifference and/or unavailability from their primary caregiver, in stressful situations, avoidantly attached infants prefer emotional and physical isolation. Anxious-ambivalent bonds are related to inconsistent, imperceptive, unresponsive, and/or enmeshed47 parenting. Here the child’s attachment system is “hyperactivated,” resulting in anxious clinging to the parent (intense “proximity-seeking”), and angry, distressed, frightened, and inconsolable affect. Later research also identified a fourth “disorganized/disorienting” category of attachment48 that appears to result from frightened or highly disoriented communication from the parent. A disorganized bond points to a combination of “deactivation” and “hyperactivation” in the infant’s attachment system, and is marked by chaotic, trance-like, and/or self-injurious behaviors. The basic attachment categories outlined above provide interpersonal neurobiologists with a theoretical construct to describe how “relationships become biological structure.”49 Along with gene expression, early communications between parent and infant “literally shape the structure of the child’s developing brain.”50 In the simplest of interactions between a parent and baby—for example, a game of peek-a-boo—interpersonal experience is being “transducted” into biology. Each instance of attuned51 communication contributes to the creation of key structures and systems in the baby’s rapidly developing brain. The neural circuitries responsible for organizing one’s relational behaviors and “stress coping capacities” throughout life are formed in and through the countless verbal and nonverbal interactions that transpire between a parent and child during the infant and toddler years.52 IPNB emphasizes that, on the one hand, secure internal working models of attachment are related to greater capacities for self-regulation, empathic attunement with others, self-love, abilities to form coherent life narratives, and expectations of positive outcomes. On the other hand, insecure attachment schema are associated with difficulties in regulating emotions and impulses, problems in maintaining memory for future consequences, struggles in keeping long-term goals in mind, low problem-solving abilities, high levels of internalized shame, poor memory and planning, incoherent and brief life narratives, and difficulties in empathizing with the needs and perspectives of others. Although attachment patterns are consistent from childhood to adulthood and tend to be transmitted generationally, as we will see, there is growing evidence that attachment schema can be changed for the better through loving, attuned relationships with others and conscious control of attentional processes.53 Neuroplasticity Fourth, IPNB assumes that neural change occurs throughout the lifespan. Several decades ago, there was general scientific consensus that lower brain and neocortical areas were unchangeable after early child development. While experiences with attachment figures in infancy and childhood do have a disproportionate effect on the growth and development of neural systems, more recent research suggests that the human brain is endowed with a lifelong ability to restructure itself with each new experience. Interpersonal neurobiologists thus maintain a constant emphasis on “the change[s] in neural connectivity induced by experience.”54 Because the brain is “not a fully formed structure but… a dynamic process undergoing constant development and reconstruction across the lifespan,”55 interpersonal neurobiologists are generally optimistic about ways in which attuned human relationships can, at any point in the life cycle, function as contexts in which positive neurological changes can unfold in the brain.56 Although it is only possible to make tentative claims at this point because the extent of the brain’s ongoing plasticity is not definitively known, “Those of us who study interpersonal neurobiology believe that friendships, marriage, psychotherapy—in fact, any meaningful relationship—can reactivate neuroplastic processes and actually change the structure of the brain.”57 Neural integration—the connecting of dissociated brain networks to form a functional whole—appears to be the manner by which brains change for the better. Along with loving and trusting interpersonal relationships, one of the most important factors in brain integration is the intentional use of executive forms of attention to notice and become attuned with one’s own internal states (e.g., fears, memories, anticipations, bodily sensations, etc.). Focused self-awareness—what Siegel calls “intrapersonal attunement” or “mindfulness”—involves “paying attention, in the present moment, on purpose, without grasping onto judgments.”58 IPNB claims that nonjudgmental self-reflexivity may facilitate integration in the adult brain by changing previously automatic modes of neural firing, thereby allowing new patterns of neural activation to emerge via the processes of synaptogenesis59 and neurogenesis.60 In other words, it appears that employing conscious control of attentional processes with the goal of resonating or attuning with different aspects of the self may stimulate specific areas of the middle prefrontal cortex to become active in ways that encourage integration. Furthermore, there is evidence to indicate that the effects of mindfulness practices on the brain are strikingly similar to those of attuned relationships; that is, the same areas of the prefrontal cortex are strengthened whether one is experiencing empathic connection with oneself or with another human being.61 This close neurobiological link between intrapersonal and interpersonal resonance in the cultivation of overall well-being becomes especially important when the focus is narrowed on questions of empathy and compassion. The Neuroscience of Empathy and Compassion It is perhaps natural for many Westerners to think of compassion as either a relatively fixed emotional and/or behavioral state, or a virtue that is possessed by individuals in varying degrees. It may strike us as somewhat odd to conceive of compassion as a trainable skill; yet, that is precisely the view that is now emerging in some parts of the neuroscientific community. This is particularly the case among researchers whose work focuses on the intersections between meditative traditions, compassion, and brain plasticity.62 Not surprisingly, interpersonal neurobiologists build on these (and other) research trends, and speak of compassion as a skill that can be intentionally built up through the activation of neuroplastic brain processes. In this section I discuss several of the current scientific research developments on human morality, empathy, and compassion from which IPNB implicitly or explicitly draws, or to which IPNB is indirectly or directly related. Mirror and Resonance Systems In the mid 1990’s, an Italian research team studying the premotor region of a monkey’s cortex found that it had special brain cells that fired not only when the monkey ate a peanut, but when it observed another primate eating a peanut.63 These “mirror neurons” (otherwise known as “monkey-see, monkey-do” neurons) were later discovered in humans,64 leading some scientists to posit that “the human brain creates representations of others’ minds.”65 While natural selection may have originally favored mirror systems in primates because they helped in coordinating social behaviors that contributed to group survival (e.g., hunting, gathering, and migration), it is thought that, in homo sapiens, “mirror systems and resonance behaviors evolved into our ability to attune to the emotional states of others. They provide us with a visceral-emotional experience of what the other is experiencing, allowing us to know others from the inside out.”66 The recent discovery of the brain’s mirror circuitry has thus been truly groundbreaking in the search for evolutionary and biological roots of kindness in primates.67 In humans, mirror neurons are unique due largely to the fact that they are located in frontal and parietal cortical regions of the brain.68 This means that they lie at the intersection of visual, motor, and emotional processing, and are therefore able to link observation, feeling, and action. Mirror neurons also play a critical role in the human ability to feel the pain of another person, and so are at the heart of empathy and compassion.69 Specific neuronal groups have been found to specialize in and respond to distinct facial expressions, vocal tones, and bodily movements in others.70 Moreover, empathic understanding of another person’s pain appears to require the ability to reflect on our own internal states, and bring them into a ‘connected-to, but distinct-from’ relationship to the pained state of the other.71 To respond to the facial, vocal, and bodily cues from another person, translate those cues into our own embodied experience, interpret them with relative accuracy, and initiate an active, intentional response is a set of skills that emerges from the high-level integrative processing functions of the prefrontal cortex.72 Because of its ability to connect us with the emotional experiences of others and, at the same time, put us in touch with our own states and give us impetus to act in moral ways, the brain’s mirror system is integral to the experience and expression of compassion. Attachment and Altruism In the mid 1980’s, Samuel and Pearl Oliner conducted an in-depth study of Gentile rescuers of Jews during the Holocaust (comparing them with nonrescuers) in order to test their hypothesis that “there may exist something called an ‘altruistic’ personality; that is, a relatively enduring predisposition to act selflessly on behalf of others, which develops early in life.”73 Among other things, the Oliners found that rescuers tended to describe their early family relationships with caregivers as close and caring, and tended to have parents who used reason rather than physical means for discipline. They also found that rescuers were more likely than non-rescuers to report feeling a poignant, personal sense of empathy for the pain of the Jewish victims. The Oliners see a clear connection between these findings; namely, that from secure attachment relationships in early life, “more rescuers learned the satisfactions accruing from personal bonds with others,”74 and were thus more likely to be moved by the agony of an other. The Oliners’ groundbreaking study points to the importance of secure attachment relationships in early life for the formation of compassionate selfhood. In fact, the link between secure attachment relationships and empathic responses to others has been demonstrated in a good number of studies.75 IPNB’s careful consideration of the role of brain development in early life helps make neurobiological sense of this link. How so? In addition to secure attachment relationships with parents in early life, research reveals that individuals high in empathy-related responding tend to have a greater ability to regulate emotion (i.e., to have conscious control over their ability to focus and shift attention and self-soothe when under stress).76 Correlatively, individuals with insecure attachment relationships with caregivers in infancy/childhood tend to have low empathy,77 and are at greater risk for emotional disturbances associated with poor regulatory control.78 IPNB provides a way to bridge these findings and suggest implications for transformation. Because many of the structures of the prefrontal cortex and regulatory system that that are associated with high levels of emotion regulation, impulse control, and empathic response take shape in positive relational environments in infancy, it makes sense that secure bonds would promote the growth of such structures.79 It also makes sense that insecure bonds, trauma, and/or high levels of environmental stress in early life would stunt the growth of those neural structures and systems,80 leading to poor affect control and decreased ability to experience and express compassion. Interpersonal neurobiologists posit that change toward greater capacities to experience and express affect regulation (tending to self) and compassion (tending to others) involves activating neuroplastic processes via conditions and practices that encourage integration.81 The Inverse Relationship Between Fear and Compassion Another fascinating finding in the Oliners’ study concerns the close correlation between fear and non-helping. In their efforts to understand bystanders’ “failure to act” (i.e., their non-assistance of Jews in Nazi Europe), they found that “Despite their hostility toward Nazis, the majority of bystanders were overcome by fear, hopelessness, and uncertainty… Asked to describe their lives during the war, their stories are brief and overwhelmingly involved with basic survival.”82 This lends support to the idea that when we feel frightened, the chances that we will reach out and help somebody else—especially someone we consider to be outside our primary group—become very slim. Moreover, research in “terror management theory” and “mortality salience” indicates that when we sense that our lives are threatened, we are more likely to act in violent or aggressive ways toward persons who do not share our own worldviews.83 IPNB pays close attention to ways in which the brain’s “fear circuitry” shapes and is shaped by both genetic factors and social interactions. In a chapter entitled “Social Phobia: When Others Trigger Fear,” Cozolino explains how the evolutionary processes that have guided the survival and development of our species have rendered the amygdala (the brain structure most directly responsible for fear responses) fully operational even before birth, making fear perhaps the strongest early human emotion.84 Fear responses in humans are primitive, fast, and powerful; the amygdala appraises inner and outer situations in fractions of a second, initiates fight/flight responses far before conscious awareness, generalizes from specific instances of past learning, and triggers anxiety in response to a vast array of internal and external cues.85 The amygdala also acts as a “social brake,” inhibiting contact with unfamiliar “others” until their safety can be assessed.86 If fear appears to hinder compassion, and if our neural circuits are primed to make us frightened much of the time, what explains the fact that we are indeed able to disengage our primal sense of alarm, and to experience things like trust and compassion? First, it is important to note that fear and empathy may not be mutually exclusive in all cases. A study by Taylor and colleagues suggests that “fight or flight” responses may be more common in males, and that stress/threat often elicits a “tend and befriend” response in females.87 Secondly, the amygdala is kept in check by its “reciprocal relationship” with the orbital medial prefrontal cortex (OMPFC), whose job it is to inhibit the amygdala’s fear responses based on conscious awareness.88 Furthermore, the quality of our early attachments has a direct bearing on our ability to regulate fearful emotions later in life via the OMPFC-amygdala link.89 Once more, an IPNB approach reveals that a well-developed prefrontal cortex—so integral to secure relational attachments, self-regulatory abilities, successful fear modulation, and experiences and expressions of empathy—is at the very heart of compassion. Because “learning not to fear and learning to love are biologically interwoven,”90 even if early insecure attachments have biased us toward feeling threatened and self-enclosed much of the time, the brain’s ongoing plasticity means that caring relationships (interpersonal attunement)91 and mindfulness practices (intrapersonal attunement)92 hold the potential to transform us into less frightened, and more compassionate, selves. Conditions that Encourage Empathy: Toward a Spirituality of Compassion Having surveyed the main tenets of IPNB, and having looked at some specific ways that an IPNB approach illuminates the neural dynamics of human empathy and compassion, it is now possible to consider some implications of these explorations for spirituality and spiritual practices. My intention in these final sections is to indwell the “transversal space” created by considering spirituality and the neurobiology of human relationships together. In this section, I extract from IPNB four basic conditions that appear to encourage neural integration (which, as we have seen, is integrally related to the emergence of empathy). I suggest that these conditions, taken together, can form the components of a spirituality of compassion—that is, a way of relating to the sacred that fosters empathic connectedness with others in their suffering, and promotes action to ease their distress. In what follows, I first speak of a particular condition as encouraging the emergence of empathy, and only thereafter do I go on to speak of it as an element of a spirituality of compassion. As discussed in the “definitions” section above, compassion presupposes empathy; in order to connect with the suffering of others and act in ways that alleviate their distress, we must first have the capability of sharing in the states of others in self-aware ways. To jump immediately to compassion without empathy as a preliminary step would not reflect the meaning of the constructs (and the science behind them) in the most accurate possible manner. Interpersonal Attunement As we have seen, IPNB suggests that when human brains “feel felt” by other human brains—in other words, when they experience a sense of emotional attunement or resonance with another attentive individual—the concurrent activation of neuroplastic processes opens up possibilities for transformation toward greater levels of well-being, which includes increased capacities to share in the states of others while maintaining secure, regulated states of self-awareness. Interpersonal attunement is thus the first condition for the emergence of empathy in humans. Over time, secure, attuned attachments with other persons may enable us to love more and fear less, rendering us progressively more capable of sharing in the suffering of others, and increasing the likelihood that we will take action to alleviate their misery. Interpersonal attunement is also the first component of a spirituality of compassion. When our manner of relating to the sacred becomes integrated with non-threatening, face-to-face, voice-to-voice, body-to-body interactions between others and ourselves, possibilities for qualitative, holistic transformation toward more compassionate ways of being in the world may open up before us. Neurobiologically speaking, this means that the mirror circuitry, which enables us to perceive and re-present the emotional and bodily experiences of the other, is, by way of the insula, altering our own limbic and bodily states moment-by-moment to match what we perceive in the other person. This mirroring is encouraging the development of more integrated circuits across brain regions. Spiritually speaking, interpersonal attunement means that we are experiencing the encounter with the other as a mediator of the sacred. That is, we sense that the intersubjective reality of the attuned relational interaction is bringing us into contact with that which (in some sense) transcends the ordinary. Intrapersonal Attunement IPNB emphasizes that mindful awareness—intentional, non-judgmental attentiveness to our own thoughts, feelings, and bodily states in the present moment—holds potential to activate neuroplastic processes, thereby changing previously automatic patterns of fear, inflexibility, and reactivity into newly integrated patterns of calm, adaptability, and balance. Our ability to be mindful in this way appears to be directly related to our ability to experience resonance with others; intrapersonal attunement, therefore, is a second condition for the emergence of empathy in humans. Like secure attachment relationships, repeated experiences of internal resonance via mindfulness practices may expand our capacities for connection with others, guide us toward increased abilities to regulate our emotions, cause us to “feel into” the pain of others with greater depth, and lead us to desire (and work toward) the alleviation of others’ suffering. Intrapersonal attunement—which involves noticing, respecting, and loving oneself93—is also the second component of a spirituality of compassion. For many individuals, becoming aware of the flow of one’s own consciousness (i.e., meta-cognition) is a profoundly spiritual experience. Experiencing the conscious “I” as an observer of our own mental representations and bodily sensations can lead us to affirm ourselves as sacred in some sense. For many, the feeling of freedom that can result from experiencing the self as more than the sum of ever-shifting sensations, emotions, and cognitions can be extraordinarily inspiring and empowering. Thus, when we bring together IPNB and spirituality, what emerges is that relating to ourselves with care, respect, curiosity, and love appears to be central to the experience of transformation toward well-being, and a vital aspect of what it means to relate to the sacred in ways that foster empathy.94 Insofar as it is related to our capacity to ascribe worth to and bestow kindness on ourselves, recognize and honor the sacredness of others, connect with them in their suffering, and take action toward the betterment of their lives, this kind of attuned, non-judgmental self-relationality is a key component in a spirituality of compassion. Relational Safety As discussed above, human fear is swift, primal, powerful, and in many cases inversely related to compassion. Interpersonal relationships marked by conflict, insecurity, and untrustworthiness can cause us to feel threatened, emotionally disregulated, and closed-off to empathic connection with others. On the other hand, interpersonal relationships marked by mutuality, security, and fidelity can help us regulate our fear responses and open us up to empathically caring for those around us. IPNB emphasizes that learning not to fear and learning to love are mutually conditioning neurobiological realities, and that relationships of safety and trust are integral to the emergence of both.95 Due to its ability to free us from fear, open us up to receiving, and encourage neural integration, relational safety is the third condition for the emergence of empathy, and the third component of a spirituality of compassion. Spirituality often involves a deep sense of existential vulnerability, so creating safe relational spaces for communal experiences of the divine is particularly important.96 Most religious traditions have practices that involve face-to-face encounters with other human beings (e.g., instruction, confession, initiation rites, group rituals). Considering IPNB and spirituality together at the intersection of compassion leads to a specific hypothesis; namely, that safe, trust-evoking relational environments for communal spiritual practices are likely to open persons up to each other, themselves, and the sacred in such a way that, (scientifically speaking), their neuroplastic processes may be activated in ways that encourage integration, and (spiritually speaking) their entire selves may become more receptive to the sacred presence which the rite itself is believed to mediate. As we have seen, IPNB suggests that neuroplastic activation and integration may stimulate the development of brain structures and systems crucial to empathy and compassion; furthermore, in many religious traditions, openness to the sacred in collective worship contexts is thought to be closely related to becoming a more compassionate person. For these core reasons, conditions of relational safety in communal practices are fundamental to a spirituality of compassion. Shared Narratives Interpersonal neurobiologists point to storytelling as a key means of neural regulation and integration. As the brain evolved in humans, more complex neural systems and networks demanded greater levels of organization. It is hypothesized that the practices of telling and listening to narratives became an evolutionary strategy “contained within the group mind (language and culture) that allowed the brain to grow further in size and complexity.”97 Not surprisingly, research reveals strong links between mental health, emotional regulation, secure attachment, and coherent narratives. Cozolino explains: Because narratives require participation of multiple structures throughout the brain, they require us to combine, in conscious memory, our knowledge, sensations, feelings, and behaviors. In bringing together multiple functions from diverse neural networks, narratives provide the brain with a tool for both emotional and neural integration.98 Telling our own story to someone else, or listening to someone narrate his or her story, asks us to be affected by and share in the state of the hearer or speaker in such a way that we hold on to our own perspective even as we attempt to indwell the experience of the other person. Shared narratives, therefore, are the fourth condition for the emergence of empathy in humans, and constitute the fourth component of a spirituality of compassion. Stories are naturally bound up within human spirituality; speaking, reading, and/or hearing sacred narratives from our traditions are at the heart many of our experiences of the divine. When, through written or oral tradition, sacred stories become integrated in some way into our own stories (individually or communally), we often have the sense of participating in something greater than ourselves.99 The combination, therefore, of (on the one hand) neural integration and empathic connection with others and self, and (on the other hand) deep personal meaning and existential orientation, means that storytelling holds potential to raise us to greater levels of concern for the pain of others, and motivate us to stand in solidarity with those who are suffering by weaving their stories into the fabric of our own. A Case Study: The Ojibwe Talking Circle A spirituality of compassion consisting of interpersonal attunement, intrapersonal attunement, relational safety, and shared narratives provides a set of criteria by which diverse spiritual practices can be theoretically evaluated for their potential to facilitate compassion in persons and communities. To show this, I now describe the Native American Ojibwe ritual of the talking circle, and briefly discuss it through the lens of my IPNB-derived spirituality of compassion. The following analysis draws heavily from my own first-hand experiences participating in talking circles as a therapist at the Minnesota Indian Women’s Resource Center.100 It is offered as a demonstration of the evaluative theoretical promise in my proposed spirituality of compassion for making non-empirical yet scientifically-informed assessments of spiritual practices, with a special eye toward the relative likelihood of the practice encouraging the emergence of compassion among participants. Description of the Talking Circle The Anishinaabe, or Ojibwe people, are native to the Great Lakes regions of North America. Like many indigenous people, they acknowledge the continuity of life and the interrelatedness of all things through the symbol of the circle. The saying, “Giiwataawayi’ij gakina gegoo wiidookodaadiimagad bimaadiziwining” (“everything in the circle helps life”) is evidence of this core aspect of Ojibwe worldview.101 The Ojibwe are a story people, and talking circles (otherwise called peacemaking circles or healing circles) are deeply rooted in their tradition of passing on their spiritual and cultural heritage through oral practices. The purpose of the talking circle is to create a safe space for small group interaction in which personal narratives and viewpoints can be communicated in an atmosphere of authenticity and deep compassionate listening that is free from threats of judgment or condemnation.102 In talking circles, all participants have the opportunity to speak without interruption. Communication is regulated by the passing of a talking piece—usually an object from nature such as a feather—that holds special and/or sacred significance to the circle facilitator (circle keeper). Only the person with the talking piece is allowed to speak. However, talking is not required; if someone wishes to remain silent, she or he can simply pass the talking piece to the next person. After participants gather together, the circle keeper begins by leading the group in a sacred cleansing ritual known as smudging, wherein a mixture of sage, cedar, and sweetgrass is burned in a shell and passed from person to person. As it is wafted onto the body of each individual and dispersed throughout the room, the smoke is believed to cleanse the space and participants from negative energies or influences, and prepare them to speak and listen from the heart. Following the smudging ritual, the circle keeper lists the groundrules of the talking circle (discussed below), and offers brief comments about the meaning of the talking piece. She or he then sets the topic for conversation by asking an open question that invites participants to share personal stories, feelings, and/or thoughts, begins the ritual by speaking first, and passes the talking piece to the person on the left (clockwise). After two or three rounds of questions and passing, there is a time of open group dialogue without the talking piece. At the end, the circle keeper makes brief concluding comments and thanks participants for their contributions. Sometimes a closing prayer is offered to honor the Creator/Great Spirit (Kitche Manitou). The Talking Circle Through the Lens of a Spirituality of Compassion The Ojibwe talking circle is a spiritual practice that holds great potential to encourage the emergence of empathy and compassion in individuals and communities. It is one example of a spiritual practice that meets the four criteria of the IPNB-informed spirituality of compassion proposed above, and may therefore be affirmed as a way of relating to the sacred that fosters empathic connectedness with others in their suffering, and promotes action to ease their distress. The face-to-face, voice-to-voice relational interactions that talking circles create are places where dynamic, resonant, empathic communication can occur. As participants listen attentively to each other—bearing compassionate witness to (and sharing compassionately in) the joys as well as the sufferings of one another’s journeys—a sense of healing and peace often emerges in their midst. In these resonant relational spaces, neural integration may occur across participants’ brain structures and systems, and Kitche Manitou may be sensed as a transformative presence being mediated in and through the interpersonally attuned connections. As they empathically listen to one another’s stories, talking circle participants are asked to calmly notice their own responses, and be attentive to the ways in which others’ stories illuminate parts of their own. When the talking piece comes to an individual, he or she attempts to reach inward and speak clearly, spontaneously, and honestly from his or her own memory, feelings, and life journey. This kind of intrapersonal attunement may lead persons to honor the sacred in themselves, cultivate a sense of self-resonance and self-regulation, build up key areas of the prefrontal cortex, support brain integration toward greater levels of well-being, and thus encourage more self-aware, non-anxious, and compassionate ways of being in the world. A talking circle is a relaxed, relationally safe space. The groundrules for participation include: (1) Listen with respect; (2) Each person gets a chance to talk; (3) One person talks at a time; (4) Speak for yourself and not as the representative of any group; (5) No name-calling or attacking.103 The talking circle thus becomes a place for openness to oneself, others, the Creator, and Nature; self-revelation (especially around painful topics) can be risked because rejection and assault are not threats. Because such protected atmospheres nurture trusting human interactions and de-activate the brain’s fear circuitry (which is toxic to neuroplastic processes), neural integration and the emergence of empathy and compassion may become more likely therein. Sharing in one another’s spoken narratives is central to the talking circle experience, and is seen as a key ingredient in the ritual’s power to bring peace and healing to relationships with oneself, others, Earth, and Kitche Manitou. Deep, respectful, compassionate listening to stories of pain and suffering brings a sense of sacred connection one to another, and is experienced as a source of redemptive spiritual and personal transformation. As participants speak their stories out loud and resonate with the stories of others, neuroplastic processes of hemispheric, systemic, and structural integration may be activated, perhaps leading to deeper capacities to be affected by the states of others in self-aware ways, and greater inclinations toward actively reaching out to others in distress. Conclusion I have suggested that there are significant implications in IPNB for identifying conditions that foster empathic ways of being in the world, and that those conditions may be gathered together to form the elements of a spirituality of compassion by which particular spiritual practices can be theoretically evaluated for their potential to cultivate compassion persons and communities. Certainly, there are questions that remain unanswered. First, given the twenty-first century reality of de-personalized, technology-dependent communication practices, and the apparent necessity of attuned, face-to-face, voice-to-voice human interactions for brain integration and the growth of empathic capacities, how realistic is it to propose a spirituality of compassion that relies so heavily on direct, embodied, relational encounters? In our globalized and bureaucracized world, compassionate praxis often involves de-personalized, systemically-aware actions rather than literal “helping-hand” behaviors. Most of us do not have potential genocide victims living down the street whom we could take into our homes. However, other impersonal acts such as refusing to purchase clothing produced by companies that exploit workers, supporting legislation that protects persons from torture and/or war crimes, and purchasing energy-efficient vehicles may have significant, positive ramifications for others who are suffering—either now or in the future. Compassion in our world requires higher-order empathy; we must find ways to connect with others’ pain when we cannot see their faces, hear their voices, speak their languages, or know their names. How can an IPNB-informed spirituality of compassion encourage care in our increasingly alienated world? Another possible point of ambiguity concerns the fact that redemptive spiritual transformation often involves experiences of pain, darkness, and/or purgation.104 How can an IPNB-derived spirituality of compassion account for the reality that emergence so often begins with emergency? Can the conditions of interpersonal attunement, intrapersonal attunement, relational safety, and shared narratives serve as “crucibles” in which transformative spiritual experiences of darkness and loss can result in even greater capacities for genuine empathy and compassion? Finally, there is the difficulty of discussing constructs such as “well-being,” “transformation” and “positive change” in normative ways. Who gets to define what growth toward well-being looks like, and what is to say that neural integration necessarily leads to expressions of empathy and compassion that support the flourishing of life? It is quite conceivable that a member of a small, close terrorist group might describe himself as so “attuned” with self, others, and God that he has decided that, in his context, “compassion” means blowing himself up in a crowded marketplace. How can an IPNB-informed spirituality of compassion account for his way of experiencing and expressing well-being? Additionally, in light of a recent study which suggests that doctors learn to deactivate empathy responses in their brains so that they can better help their patients who are in pain,105 are there ways in which sharing in the suffering of another may sometimes weaken (rather than strengthen) one’s ability to act in ways that alleviate their distress? It seems important to acknowledge that there are times and places in which an apparently non-empathic and/or dispassionate response may in fact be the most helpful one. I do not view these points of ambiguity as detractions from my argument. Rather, I think of them as invitations for future explorations into the implications of IPNB for spirituality, transformation, and compassion. Further research is needed not only on the above questions; broadly speaking, we need more empirical studies that shed light on relationships between specific spiritual practices, neurological patterns, attachment schema, and compassionate attitudes and actions. This kind of work will require input and expertise from many different religious, scientific, philosophical, and cultural perspectives. We are thus invited toward ever-greater efforts to attune with ourselves, scholars in our own fields, researchers in other disciplines, and practitioners in diverse cultural and religious traditions in order to continue the complex, arduous, yet rewarding process of uncovering the dynamics of human transformation, and revealing the conditions of the possibility of the emergence of compassion in selves and societies.
Endnotes 1Stephen G. Post and Lynn G. Underwood, “Concluding Summary: Future Research Needs on Altruism and Altruistic Love,” in Altruism and Altruistic Love: Science, Philosophy, & Religion in Dialogue, ed. Stephen G. Post, Lynn G. Underwood, Jeffrey P. Schloss, and William B. Hurlbut (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002), 379-386; quote p. 382. 2 Lucy Bregman, “Spirituality: A Glowing and Useful Term in Search of a Meaning,” Omega: Journal of Death and Dying 53, ½ (2006): 5-26. 3 Brian J. Zinnbauer and Kenneth I. Pargament, “Religiousness and Spirituality,” in Handbook of the Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, ed. Raymond F. Paloutzian and Crystal L. Park (New York: The Guilford Press, 2005): 21-42. 4 F. LeRon Shults and Steven J. Sandage, Transforming Spirituality: Integrating Theology and Psychology (Grand Rapids, MI: Baker Academic, 2005). 5 P.C. Hill et al., “Conceptualizing Religion and Spirituality: Points of Commonality, Points of Departure,” Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 30 (2000): 51-77; quote p. 66. 6 Shults and Sandage, Transforming Spirituality, 161. 7 Cf. Lee A. Kirkpatrick, Attachment, Evolution, and the Psychology of Religion (New York: The Guilford Press, 2005); Ana-Maria Rizzuto, The Birth of the Living God: A Psychoanalytic Study (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1979); Randall Lehmann Sorenson, Minding Spirituality (Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press, 2004) 8 I define “spiritual practice” as an intentional mode of acting that is thought to mediate our relation(s) to the sacred. 9 Theodor Lipps, Empathy, Inner Imitation, and Sense-Feelings, reprinted in Harry Francis Mallgrave and Elftherious Ikonomou (eds), Empathy, Form, and Space: Problems in German Aesthetics 1873-1893 (Los Angeles: The Getty Center for the History of Art and the Humanities, 1994), 28, quoted in Susan Verducci, “A Conceptual History of Empathy and a Question it Raises for Moral Education,” Educational Theory 50, 1 (Winter 2000): 63-80; quote p. 68. 10 Susan K. Langer, Mind: An Essay on Human Feeling (Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1984). 11 De Wall’s broad construal of empathy contains within it a sort of built-in scale (or range) of evolutionary development; on the low end are unconscious mirror responses to others through body mimicry (e.g., yawning when someone else yawns), and at the high end are poignant feelings of resonance with another’s pain, and targeted helping behaviors aimed to relieve their suffering. He notes that although humans have the most evolutionarily developed capacities for empathy, other species empathize at a level close to that of humans: “Perhaps empathy has reached its peak in our species, but several other animals—most notably apes, dolphins, and elephants—come close. These animals understand the other’s predicament well enough to offer optimal assistance… They may not know the golden rule, but they surely seem to follow it.” Frans De Wall, Our Inner Ape: A Leading Primatologist Explains Why We Are Who We Are (New York: Riverhead Books, 2005), 175, 186. 12 Daniel Batson, well known for his “empathy-altruism hypothesis” which claims that “feeling empathy for a person in need evokes altruistic motivation to relieve that need,” defines empathy as “an other-oriented emotional response congruent with the perceived welfare of another.” “Addressing the Altruism Question Experimentally,” in Altruism and Altruistic Love: Science, Philosophy, & Religion in Dialogue, ed. Stephen G. Post, Lynn G. Underwood, Jeffrey P. Schloss, and William B. Hurlbut (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002), 89-105; quote p. 92. Similarly, Nancy Eisenberg defines empathy as “an affective response that stems from the apprehension or comprehension of another’s emotional state or condition, and that is similar to what the other person is feeling or would be expected to feel.” Eisenberg distinguishes empathy from sympathy. For her, sympathy is “an affective response that frequently stems from empathy, but does not consist merely of feeling the same emotion as the other is experiencing… Rather, sympathy consists of feelings of sorrow or concern for the distressed or needy other.” “Empathy-Related Emotional Responses, Altruism, and Their Socialization,” in Visions of Compassion: Western Scientists and Tibetan Buddhists Examine Human Nature, ed. Richard J. Davidson and Anne Harrington (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002), 131-164; quote p. 135. 13 Antonio R. Damasio, after describing his view of “emotions” as part of a complex set of processes involving neural and chemical responses initiated in the brain and forming patterns throughout the body, states: “Such processes [i.e., emotions] include the ability to sense not just states of the self but also states of the other. That sense is also known as empathy. The ability to reason and decide in a social realm and the ability to be intelligent and creative in a social realm depend, to a considerable part, on this combined knowledge of self and other.” “A Note on the Neurobiology of Emotions,” in Post and Underwood, Altruism and Altruistic Love, 264-271; quote p. 270. For J. Decety and P.L. Jackson, empathy consists of three primary elements: first, a sharing of another’s affect; second, the maintenance of a separate self-representation; third, flexible mechanisms of emotional self-regulation to allow engagement of the differential perspectives of self and other. “The functional architecture of human empathy,” Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 3, 2 (2004): 71-100. Singer et al state that empathy “broadly refers to being able to understand what others feel, be it an emotion or a sensory state. Accordingly, empathic experience enables us to understand what it feels like when someone else experiences sadness or happiness, and also pain, touch, or tickling.” T. Singer, B. Seymour, J. O’Doherty, H. Kaube, R. Dolan, and C. Frith, “Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but not Sensory Components of Pain,” Science 303, 5661 (Feb. 2004): 1157-1162; quote p. 1157. 14 Louis Cozolino, The Neuroscience of Human Relationships: Attachment and the Developing Social Brain (New York: W.W. Norton and Company, 2006), 19 (hereafter NHR); Daniel J. Siegel and Mary Hartzell. Parenting from the Inside Out: How a Deeper Self-Understanding Can Help You Raise Children Who Thrive (New York: Penguin, 2003), 224. 15 Self-differentiation (or differentiation of self) is a hallmark of family systems theory. In essence, it involves the ability to hold onto oneself while also connecting meaningfully with others. The classic texts on self-differentiation are: Murray Bowen, Family Therapy in Clinical Practice (Lanham, Maryland: Rowman and Littlefield, 2004. First published 1978 by Jason Aronson, Inc.); Michel E. Kerr and Murray Bowen, Family Evaluation: An Approach Based on Bowen Theory (New York: W.W. Norton and Company, 1988). For a more contemporary treatment of self-differentiation in the context of marital and sexual therapy, see David Schnarch’s books: Constructing the Sexual Crucible: An Integration of Sexual and Marital Therapy (New York: W.W. Norton and Company, 1991); Passionate Marriage: Keeping Love and Intimacy Alive in Committed Relationships (New York: Henry Holt and Company, 1997). 16 Henri Nouwen, Donald P. McNeill, and Douglas A. Morrison, Compassion: A Reflection on the Christian Life (New York: Doubleday, 1982), 27. 17 His Holiness the Dalai Lama, “Understanding Our Fundamental Nature,” in Davidson and Harrington, Visions of Compassion, 66-80; quote p. 73. 18 Patrick R. Steffen and Kevin S. Masters, “Does Compassion Mediate the Intrinsic Religion-Health Relationship?” Annals of Behavioral Medicine 30, 3 (2005): 217-224; quote p. 218. 19 J. A. King, R. J. R. Blair, D. G. V. Mitchell, R. J. Dolan, and N. Burgess, “Doing the Right Thing: A Common Neural Circuit for Appropriate Violent or Compassionate Behavior,” NeuroImage 30 (2006): 1069-1076; quote p. 1070. 20 My conception of “helping behavior” is broad; while concrete actions such as feeding, clothing, washing, or nursing another person are obvious examples, I also consider the simple act of empathically bearing witness to another’s pain a “helping action.” As Anne Harrington (Professor of the History of Science at Harvard University) has pointed out, when we worry too much about whether empathy or compassion are translated into actual helping behavior, we are often tempted to overlook opportunities for deeper understandings of the role of compassion in human life: “I think in some ways, knowing that a person is compassionately watching your suffering, bearing witness, is helpful in itself even if no action results.” Anne Harrington et al., “Dialogues, Part I: Fundamental Questions,” in Davidson and Harrington, Visions of Compassion, 81-103; quote p. 101. 21 His Holiness the Dalai Lama, “Dialogues, Part II: Pragmatic Extensions and Applications,” in Davidson and Harrington, Visions of Compassion, 213-246. 22 There is empirical evidence to indicate that compassionate attitudes and behaviors are linked with positive health outcomes (including reduced depressive symptoms and reduced perceived stress); however, there is also evidence that compassionate behaviors alone (i.e., without corresponding compassionate attitudes) are not enough to engender health benefits. For example, in their study on the role of compassion in the religion-health relationship, Steffen and Masters found evidence that “compassionate behaviors only relate to better health functioning when they are accompanied by internalized compassionate attitudes.” “Does Compassion Mediate the Intrinsic Religion-Health Relationship?” 222. Similarly, Lynn G. Underwood, who studies factors affecting health and illness in human populations, finds that there are motives that detract from the quality of compassionate love. These include the need for reciprocal love and affection, the need to be accepted by others or by God, the need to belong, guilt, fear, seeing the other as an extension or reflection of oneself, pleasure in looking well in the eyes of others, control of the other through their indebtedness, desire to exercise power over others, desire to reinforce a positive image of self and/or feelings of superiority, and a desire to avoid confrontation. “The Human Experience of Compassionate Love: Conceptual Mapping and Data from Selected Studies,” in Post et al., Altruism and Altruistic Love, 72-105. 23 Cf. J. Wentzel van Huyssteen, Alone in the World? Human Uniqueness in Science and Theology. The Gifford Lectures, University of Edinburgh, Spring, 2004. (Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans, 2006); J. Wentzel van Huyssteen, The Shaping of Rationality: Toward Interdisciplinarity in Theology and Science (Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans, 1999); F. LeRon Shults, The Postfoundationalist Task of Theology: Wolfhart Pannenberg and the New Theological Rationality (Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans, 1999); Mikael Stenmark, How to Relate Science and Religion: A Multidimentional Model (Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans, 2004). 24 Cf. Calvin Schrag, “Transversal Rationality,” in The Question of Hermeneutics: Essays in Honor of Joseph J. Kockelmans, ed. T. J. Stapleton (Dordrecht: Kluwer, 1994). 25 van Huyssteen, The Shaping of Rationality, 247. 26 While some references will be made in this paper to specific scientific studies that inform IPNB, as a general rule I will focus on IPNB scholarship, which synthesizes the relevant primary source material. This is due to (first) the vastness and diversity of the literature on which IPNB draws, and (second) the overall goal of this essay, which is to connect IPNB itself with thoughts on spirituality, empathy, and compassion. An implication of this, of course, is that my argument is prone to the critiques to which IPNB is prone. As a fairly new transdisciplinary field, IPNB has not yet been the subject of significant critical evaluation. This is not likely to remain the case indefinitely; moreover, we can anticipate that criticism will likely come from two different philosophical/methodological perspectives, broadly speaking. On the one hand, those who approach the study of the human mind reductionistically may be suspicious of the IPNB concept of “social synapses” (Cozolino 2006, 5), which seems to point toward the idea of “extended mind.” Reductionists may also be nervous about the notion of “intrapersonal attunement” in which “the mind [uses] the brain to create itself” (Siegel 2007, 32), which appears to suggest a sort of “top-down causality” that is typical of emergence theories. On the other hand, those who hold to a dualistic anthropology in which body and soul (or the “material” and the “spiritual”) are seen as separate, distinct substances may be dissatisfied with IPNB’s focus on the interface between brain functioning and human relationality, to the exclusion of discussions on “immaterial” phenomena. Dualists may also worry about the direct link IPNB makes between “neural integration” in the human brain and overall well-being. While space does not permit a treatment of these philosophical and methodological issues, suffice it to say that most IPNB scholarship to this point seems to presuppose a kind of “middle ground” between reductionism and dualism. 29 Neurons are cells that transmit signals to one another via chemical messengers. They are the basic unit of the nervous system. 32 “The social synapse is the space between us. It is also the medium through which we are linked together into larger organisms such as families, tribes, societies, and the human species as a whole.” Cozolino, NHR, 5. 33 Daniel J. Siegel, The Mindful Brain: Reflection and Attunement in the Cultivation of Well-Being (New York: W.W. Norton and Company, 2007), 5. 34 Daniel J. Siegel, “An Interpersonal Neurobiology Approach to Psychotherapy,” Psychiatric Annals 36, 4 (April 2006): 248-256; quote p. 248. 35 Siegel, The Mindful Brain, 26; cf. pp. 341-345. 37 Allan N. Schore, Affect Dysregulation and Disorders of the Self (New York: W.W. Norton and Company, 2003). 38 These and other brain structures are discussed in Cozolino, NHR, 51-57. 39 For our purposes, the most important regulatory systems are the mirror and resonance systems, which play a key role in “empathic attunement.” Mirror circuitry is discussed below. 40 Siegel, The Developing Mind, 8. 41 The complexities of the processes of neural integration render it an extremely difficult concept to describe. Cozolino’s description is the clearest I’ve come across: “Integration of neural networks involves the flow of information through efficient interconnections and a balance of influences from participating systems. Directions of integration include top-down (cortical to subcortical and back again) and left-right (across the two hemispheres of the brain). Top-down integration includes the ability of the cortex to process, inhibit, and organize the reflexes, impulses, and emotions generated by the brainstem and limbic system… Left-right integration allows us to put feelings into words, consider feelings in conscious awareness, and integrate the positive and negative affective biases of the left and right hemispheres” (NHR, 43). 42 Cf. John Bowlby, Attachment and Loss. Volume 1: Attachment, 2nd ed. (1969; New York: Basic Books, 1982); A Secure Base: Parent-child Attachments and Healthy Human Development (New York: Basic Books, 1988). 43 Cf. C. Hazan and P.R. Shaver, “Romantic Love Conceptualized as an Attachment Process,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 (1987): 511-524. 44 For an accessible introduction to attachment theory and research, see Robert Karen, Becoming Attached: First Relationships and How They Shape Our Capacity to Love (Warner Books, 1994; Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1998). For a literature review of adult attachment studies and a discussion of their significance for a description of the “healthy, effective self,” see Frederick G. Lopez and Kelly A. Brennan, “Dynamic Processes Underlying Adult Attachment Organization: Toward an Attachment Theoretical Perspective on the Healthy and Effective Self,” Journal of Counseling Psychology 47, 3 (2000): 283-300. 45 Most notably, Mary Ainsworth and her colleagues. Cf. M.D.S. Ainsworth, M. Blehar, E. Waters and S. Wall, Patterns of Attachment: A Study of the Strange Situation (Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum, 1978). 46 Lopez and Brennan, “Dynamic Processes Underlying Adult Attachment Organization,” 284. 47 In enmeshed parenting, the caregiver intrudes his or her anxious state of mind onto the mind of the child, resulting in the child’s internalization of the parent’s inner distress. 48 M. Main and J. Solomon, “Procedures for Identifying Infants as Disorganized/Disoriented During the Ainsworth Strange Situation,” in Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, Research, and Intervention, ed. M.T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, and E.M. Cummings (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1990), 121-160. 50 Daniel J. Siegel, The Developing Mind: How Relationships and the Brain Interact to Shape Who We Are (New York: The Guilford Press, 1999), 21. 51 Attunement (or resonance) is the ability to feel another person’s emotions. “Feeling felt” is the subjective experience of attuned human interactions. Siegel notes: “The pleasurable response to such a resonance of minds may be built into our brains as a genetic inheritance of evolutionary history. For us as social animals, our having such a sense encourages group behavior, which has been of great survival value to our species as we evolved.” The Developing Mind, 149. 52 Schore, Affect Dysregulation. 54 Siegel, “An Interpersonal Neurobiology Approach to Psychotherapy,” 250. 56 A study conducted in 2005 by Sara Lazar and her colleagues—one cited frequently by Siegel—appears to provide striking evidence for the adult brain’s ability to undergo significant change in prefrontal cortical areas in response to specific practices. The study found that individuals who had engaged in mindfulness meditation practices for extended periods of time had thicker middle prefrontal areas and right insulas than non-meditators, and that the thickness in these regions was correlated with length of time spent in mindfulness meditation. This research lends support to the idea that mindfulness practices have the potential to transform and solidify neural patterns in ways that promote overall well-being. Siegel, The Mindful Brain, 341-345; cf. S.W. Lazar, C.E. Kerr, R.H. Wasserman, J.R. Gray, D.N. Greve, M.T. Treaday, et al, “Meditation Experience is Associated with Increased Cortical Thickness,” Neuroreport 16, 17 (2005): 1893-1897. 58 Siegel, “An Interpersonal Neurobiology Approach to Psychotherapy,” 250. Siegel’s definition of mindfulness closely resembles the definition offered by Jon Kabat-Zinn, who is renowned for bringing mindfulness meditation into the mainstream of modern medicine and society. In Kabat-Zinn’s view, “An operational working definition of mindfulness is: the awareness that emerges through paying attention, on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally to the unfolding of experience moment by moment.” Jon Kabat-Zinn, “Mindfulness-Based Interventions in Context: Past, Present, and Future,” Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 10, 2 (2003): 144-156; quote p. 145. Cf. Kabat-Zinn, Coming to Our Senses: Healing Ourselves and the World Through Mindfulness (New York: Hyperion, 2005). 59 The creation of new neural synaptic linkages. 60 The creation of new brain cells. 61 The intriguing neuroscientific confluence between research in attachment and mindfulness is at the heart of Siegel’s most recent work, The Mindful Brain. 62 “Many of our core mental processes such as awareness and attention and emotion regulation, including our very capacity for happiness and compassion, should best be conceptualized as trainable skills. The meditative traditions provide a compelling example of strategies and techniques that have evolved over time to enhance and optimize human potential and well-being. The neuroscientific study of these traditions is still in its infancy but the early findings promise to both reveal the mechanisms by which such training may exert its effects as well as underscore the plasticity of the brain circuits that underlie complex mental functions.” A. Lutz, J.D. Dunne and R.J Davidson, “Meditation and the Neuroscience of Consciousness: An Introduction,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness, ed. P.D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2007), 499-554, quoted in Siegel, The Mindful Brain, 101-102. 63 V. Gallese, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, and G. Rizzolatti, “Action Recognition in the Premotor Cortex,” Brain 119 (1996): 593-609. 64 M. Iacoboni, R.P. Woods, M. Brass, H. Bekkering, J.C Maziotta, and G. Rizzolatti, “Cortical Mechanisms of Human Imitation,” Science 286, 5449 (1999): 2526-2528; M. Iacoboni, L.M. Koski, M. Brass, H. Bekkering, R.P. Woods, M.C. Dubeau, J.C. Maziotta, and G. Rizzolatti, “Reafferent Copies of Imitated Actions in the Right Superior Temporal Cortex,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98, 24 (2001): 13995-13999. 65 Siegel, The Mindful Brain, 166. 67 Primatologist Frans De Wall sums the meaning of the finding of mirror neurons thus: “Social animals relate to each other at a level far more basic than scientists previously suspected. We are hardwired to connect with those around us and to resonate with them, also emotionally. It’s a fully automated process.” De Waal, Our Inner Ape, 177. It is widely accepted in the scientific community that altruism is closely tied to evolutionary dynamics, and has aided in the flourishing of our species. Cf. Michael Ruse, “A Darwinian Naturalist’s Perspective on Altruism,” in Altruism and Altruistic Love, 151-167; Elliot Sober, “Kindness and Cruelty in Evolution,” in Visions of Compassion, 46-65. 68 Cozolino summarizes the specific areas of the brain involved in mirroring: “Current research supports that the human mirror system extends to the temporal, parietal, and frontal lobes as well as to the insula, amygdala, basal ganglia, and cerebellum. The areas of the brain that become activated depend on the task and whether it is observed, imagined, or involves emotions.” NHR, 193. 69 L. Carr, M. Iacoboni, M-C Dubeau, J.C. Maziotta, and G. L. Lenzi, “Neural Mechanisms of Empathy in Humans: A Relay from Neural Systems for Imitation to Limbic Areas,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 9 (2003): 5497-5502; V. Gallese, “The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity,” Psychopathology 36, 4 (2003): 171-180. 71 Carr and colleagues propose a 3-step “insula-hypothesis” model of empathy. According to them, empathic response consists of, first, “interoception,” which relays data from our limbic and bodily areas via the insula in order to sense what we are feeling in our own body in response to another’s pain; second, “interpretation,” which involves making sense of our state shifts; third, “attribution,” which consists of attributing our own experience onto the experience of the other. Carr et al., “Neural Mechanisms of Empathy in Humans.” 72 A wealth of psychiatric and neuroscientific research has confirmed that prefrontal cortical areas are integrally involved in human empathy, morality, and compassion. Many of these studies focus on the results of damage to prefrontal areas, or abnormalities in prefrontal functioning in individuals who have been diagnosed with social psychiatric disorders. Cf. M. Dolan, “What Neuroimaging Tells us About Psychopathic Disorders,” Hospital Medicine 32 (2002): 417-427; King et al.,“Doing the Right Thing”; Michael Koenigs, Lian Young, Ralph Adolphs, Daniel Tranel, Fiery Cushman, Marc Hauser, and Antonio Damasio, “Damage to the Prefrontal Cortex Increases Utilitarian Moral Judgments.” Nature 446 (April 2007): 908-911; J. L Müller, M. Sommer, V. Wagner, K. Lange, H. Taschler, C.H. Röder et. al., “Abnormalities in Emotion Processing Within Cortical and Subcortical Regions in Criminal Psychopaths: Evidence From a Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study Using Pictures With Emotional Content,” Biological Psychiatry 54 (2003): 152-162. 73 Samuel P. Oliner and Pearl M. Oliner. The Altruistic Personality: Rescuers of Jews in Nazi Europe. New York: The Free Press, 1988, 3. 74 Oliner and Oliner, The Altruistic Personality, 173. It is interesting to note that Elliot Sober, in his attempt to make evolutionary sense of “extended compassion” (i.e., feeling compassion toward strangers and other species), points to the Oliners’ study as evidence that the evolutionarily advantageous trait of feeling compassion toward one’s children (i.e., secure attachment relationships) have certain side effects, and that extended compassion is one of them. “Individuals well attuned to the suffering of those near and dear have circles of compassion that potentially extend quite far afield.” Elliot Sober, “Kindness and Cruelty in Evolution,” 63. 75 E. Waters, J. Wippman and L.A. Sroufe found that children with secure attachment bonds with primary caregivers (versus children with insecure bonds) are apt to be sympathetic with peers at age 3 ½. “Attachment, Positive Affect, and Competence in the Peer Group: Two Studies in construct Validation,” Child Development 50 (1979): 821-829. Likewise, R. Kestenbaum, E.A. Farber, and L.A. Stroufe found that securely attached children show high levels of empathic and prosocial behaviors in preschool. “Individual Differences in Empathy Among Preschoolers: Relation to Attachment History,” New Directions in Child Development 44 (1989): 51-64. Research findings also support the hypothesis that authoritative parenting (i.e., warm, democratic, expectant of mature behavior from their children, and generous in providing information to their children), versus authoritarian parenting (i.e., low in warmth, directive, negative) is linked with offspring’s empathy and sympathy. M. Dekovic and J.M.A.M. Janssens, “Parents’ Child-Rearing Style and Children’s Sociometric Status,” Developmental Psychology 28 (1992): 925-932; J. Krevand and J.C. Gibbs, “Parents’ Use of Inductive Discipline: Relations to Children’s Empathy and Prosocial Behavior,” Child Development 67 (1996): 3263-3277. 76 N. Eisenberg, M. Wentzel, and J.D. Harris, “The Role of Emotionality and Regulation in Empathy-Related Responding,” School Psychology Review 27 (1998): 506-521. 77 M. Main and C. George, “Responses of Abused and Disadvantaged Toddlers to Distress in Agemates: A Study in the Day Care Setting,” Developmental Psychology 21 (1985): 407-412; P. Miller and N. Eisenberg, “The Relation of Empathy to Aggression and Externalizing/Antisocial Behavior,” Psychological Bulletin 103 (1988): 324-344. 78 R. Karr-Morse and M.S. Wiley, Ghosts From the Nursery: Tracing the Roots of Violence (New York: Atlantic Monthly Press, 1997); B. D. Perry, “Incubated in Terror: Neurodevelopmental Factors in the ‘Cycle of Violence,’” in Children in a Violent Society, ed. J. Osofsky (New York: The Guilford Press, 1997), 124-149. 79 Cozolino, NHR; Schore, Affect Dysregulation; Allan N. Schore, Affect Regulation and the Repair of the Self (New York: W.W. Norton, 2003); Siegel, The Mindful Brain. 80 “‘Developmental overpruning’ refers to a toxic effect of overwhelming stress on the young brain: The release of stress hormones leads to excessive death of neurons in the crucial pathways involving the neocortex and limbic system—the areas responsible for emotional regulation.” Siegel, The Developing Mind, 85. Cf. Pat Ogden, Kekuni Minton, and Clare Pain, Trauma and the Body: A Sensorimotor Approach to Psychotherapy (New York: W.W. Norton and Company, 2006); Schore, Affect Dysregulation; Onno van der Hart, Ellert R.S. Nijenhuis, and Kathy Steele. The Haunted Self: Structural Dissociation and the Treatment of Chronic Traumatization (New York: W.W. Norton and Company, 2006). 81 These conditions and practices will be discussed in greater detail in the next section. 82 Oliner and Oliner, The Altruistic Personality, 146. 83 Holly A. McGregor, Joel D. Lieberman, Jeff Geenberg, Sheldon Solomon, Jamie Arndt, Linda Simon, and Tom Pyszczynaski, “Terror Management and Aggression: Evidence That Mortality Salience Motivates Aggression Against Worldview-Threatening Others,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74, 3 (1998): 590-605. 86 There is evidence, in fact, that “the neurobiology of racism… [is] related to the fear circuitry of the brain.” Cozolino, NHR, 253; Cf. E.A. Phelps, K.J. O’Connor, W.A. Cunningham, E.S. Funayama, J.C. Gatenby, J.C. Gore, “Performance on Indirect Measures of Race Evaluation Predicts Amygdala Activation,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12 (2000): 729-738; E.A. Phelps, and L.A. Thomas, “Race, Behavior, and the Brain: The Role of Neuroimaging in Understanding Complex Social Behaviors,” Political Psychology 24 (2003): 747-758. 87 S.E. Taylor, L.C. Klein, B.P. Lewis, T.L. Gruenewald, R.A.R. Gurung, and J.A. Updegraff, “Biobehavioral Responses to Stress in Females: Tend-and-Befriend, not Fight-or-Flight,” Psychological Review 107, 3 (2000): 411-429. 88 Cozolino, NHR, 60; Cf. J.S. Beer, E.A. Heerey, D. Keltner, D. Scabini, and R.T. Knight, “The Regulatory Function of Self-Conscious Emotion: Insights From Patients With Orbitofrontal Damage,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85 (2003): 594-604. 89 Cozolino explains, “Since the networks connecting the OMPFC and the amygdala are shaped by experience, our learning history of what is safe and dangerous, including our attachment schema, are thought to be encoded within this system.” NHR, 60. 90 Cozolino, NHR, 314. Cf. A. Bartels and S. Zeki, “Functional Brain Mapping During Free Viewing of Natural Scenes,” Human Brain Mapping 21 (2003): 75-83. 91 “Kindness pulls the rug out from under the defenses we have learned to use to protect ourselves from feelings of rejection and abandonment. Put in another way, compassion, warmth, and love have the power to change our brains.” Cozolino, NHR, 315. 92 “In the intensely activated neural structures of threat, our limbic regions influence cortical reasoning and we come to believe, without a doubt, that we are right in our assessments. And ‘they’ are wrong. When the stakes are high in these intense times of tyranny and technological advance, a mindful awareness of these neural mechanisms and the reflection necessary to disengage from their automatic reactions are desperately needed. Reflection is no longer a luxury, it may be a necessity for our survival.” Siegel, The Mindful Brain, 324. Cf. D. Derryberry and M.A. Reed, “Anxiety-Related Attentional Biases and their Regulation by Attentional Control,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology 111 (2002): 225-236. 93 Kristin Neff’s notion of “self-compassion” holds close affinities with Siegel’s concept of intrapersonal attunement, and may be another window into a core component of a spirituality of compassion. “Self-Compassion: An Alternative Conceptualization of Healthy Attitude Toward Oneself,” Self and Identity 2 (2003): 85-101. 94 Correlatively, spiritualities that foster shame or self-condemnation may impede the development of empathic, compassionate feeling and praxis. 95 Cozolino, in elucidating the components of psychotherapy that optimize neuroplasticity and create the possibility for changing the brain, includes “a safe and trusting relationship with an attuned therapist” as a condition for the possibility of patients’ neural integration and transformation. The Neuroscience of Psychotherapy: Building and Rebuilding the Human Brain (New York: W.W. Norton and Company, 2002). Similarly, Siegel (building on Steven W. Porges’ research) posits that resonance between two people creates a “neuroception of safety” in the brain which results in a state of open receptivity marked by “the softening of facial muscles, relaxation in vocal tone, and opening of the perceptual system to receive input from outside the self.” This neuroceptive assessment of safety is also called “love without fear,” and Siegel believes it is integrally related to both interpersonal and intrapersonal attunement, and a key ingredient in neural integration. The Mindful Brain, 129-131. Cf. S. W. Porges, “Love: An Emergent Property of the Mammalian Autonomic Nervous System,” Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 8 (1998): 837-861. 96 I have found the following list of groundrules to be quite reliable for establishing relational safety in contexts of communal spiritual practices: (1) Presume welcome and extend welcome. (2) Refrain from fixing, saving, or setting straight others in the group. (3) When the interaction gets tricky, turn to inquiry rather than advocacy (wonder about something instead of defending something). (4) Ask open, honest questions. Open questions are ones to which you cannot imagine “the right answer” and which have several possible responses; honest questions are ones which do not have a hidden agenda. (5) Speak for yourself (this will require listening to yourself). (6) Think of silence as another member of the group. (7) Observe confidentiality regarding material shared in the group. Adapted from Carla M. Dahl, “Guidelines for Group Interaction in Marriage and Family Therapy Courses and Formation Experiences” (St. Paul, MN: Bethel Seminary, 2003). 99 Cf. James L. Griffith and Melissa Elliott Griffith, Encountering the Sacred in Psychotherapy: How to Talk With People About Their Spiritual Lives (New York: The Guilford Press, 2002). 100 www.miwrc.org. Talking circles may be practiced in slightly different ways in different contexts; my description indicates the way it is often practiced at MIWRC. 101 For an introduction to Ojibwe spirituality, culture, and history, cf. Thomas Peacock and Marlene Wisuri, Ojibwe Waasa Inaabidaa: We Look in All Directions (Afton, MN: Afton Historical Society Press, 2002). 102 Mark Umbreit, “Talking Circles,” article written for the Center for Restorative Justice and Peacemaking (University of Minnesota, Aug. 2003), http://rjp.umn.edu/Copy_of_Restorative_Justice_Princples.html (accessed December 7, 2007). 103 Umbreit, “Talking Circles.” 104 For example, in his Terror and Transformation, clinical psychologist and philosopher of religion James Jones draws on the rich apophatic traditions in Christian spirituality, and presents the via negativa (the negative way of unknowing) as an important aspect of religious de-idealization and redemptive change. Terror and Transformation: The Ambiguity of Religion in Psychoanalytic Perspective (New York: Brunner-Routledge, 2002). In a similar vein, psychologist Steven J. Sandage discusses the ways in which dark “crucibles” of intense spiritual negation can function as places of transformation in which one is faced with the opportunity to re-figure relationships with self, others and God in ways that “can strengthen both the security of attachment and the wholeness of differentiation.” Shults and Sandage, Transforming Spirituality, 241. 105 Y. Cheng, C. Lin, H.L. Liu, Y. Hsu, D. Hung, and J. Decety, “Expertise Modulates the Perception of Pain in Others,” Current Biology 17 (2007): 1708-1713.
Published
2008.05.22
|
|
|
|
|